Agriculture
Watching out for vultures
Fast-flying fungal spores
Making the most of a meal
Amphibians
Tree Frogs
Bullfrogs
Newts
Animals
Walktopus
Koalas, Up Close and Personal
Vent Worms Like It Hot
Behavior
Storing Memories before Bedtime
Primate Memory Showdown
Talking with Hands
Birds
Peafowl
Albatrosses
Storks
Chemistry and Materials
Screaming for Ice Cream
Spinning Clay into Cotton
Makeup Science
Computers
The hungry blob at the edge of the universe
A New Look at Saturn's rings
Lighting goes digital
Dinosaurs and Fossils
The man who rocked biology to its core
Dinosaur Dig
Dino-bite!
E Learning Jamaica
Results of GSAT are in schools this week
E Learning in Jamaica WIN PRIZES and try our Fun Animated Games
2014 GSAT Results for Jamaican Kids
Earth
Killer Space Rock Snuffed Out Ancient Life
Challenging the Forces of Nature
Snowflakes and Avalanches
Environment
A Newspaper's Hidden Cost
The Birds are Falling
Improving the Camel
Finding the Past
Traces of Ancient Campfires
A Plankhouse Past
Digging Up Stone Age Art
Fish
A Grim Future for Some Killer Whales
Lungfish
A Jellyfish's Blurry View
Food and Nutrition
A Taste for Cheese
Sponges' secret weapon
The Color of Health
GSAT English Rules
Pronouns
Whoever vs. Whomever
Who vs. That vs. Which
GSAT Exam Preparation Jamaica
How are students placed after passing the GSAT exam
March 21-22, 2013: Over 43,000 students will take the GSAT Exam
Preparing for the GSAT Exam
GSAT Exams Jamaica Scholarships
42,000 students will sit for the GSAT Exam in two weeks
Results of GSAT are in schools this week
GSAT stars reap scholarship glory
GSAT Mathematics
42,000 students will sit for the GSAT Exam in two weeks
Secrets of an Ancient Computer
Setting a Prime Number Record
Human Body
Football Scrapes and Nasty Infections
What the appendix is good for
Sea Kids See Clearly Underwater
Invertebrates
Worms
Beetles
Spiders
Mammals
Bulldogs
Chipmunks
Cocker Spaniels
Parents
How children learn
What Not to Say to Emerging Readers
Expert report highlights the importance to parents of reading to children!
Physics
Hold on to your stars, ladies and gentlemen
Powering Ball Lightning
Echoes of a Stretched Egg
Plants
Nature's Alphabet
Sweet, Sticky Science
Stalking Plants by Scent
Reptiles
Iguanas
Box Turtles
Cobras
Space and Astronomy
Melting Snow on Mars
Tossing Out a Black Hole Life Preserver
A Smashing Display
Technology and Engineering
Algae Motors
Squeezing Oil from Old Wells
Reach for the Sky
The Parts of Speech
What is a Preposition?
Adjectives and Adverbs
What is a Noun
Transportation
Middle school science adventures
Tinkering With the Basic Bike
Flying the Hyper Skies
Weather
The solar system's biggest junkyard
Weekend Weather Really Is Different
In Antarctica watch the heat (and your step)
Add your Article

Wired for Math

Quick. What's 845 + 289? Sorry, time's up. Few people can add numbers in their heads that quickly. If someone showed you a set of 845 dots next to a set of 289 dots, however, you'd probably be able to tell right away which set has more dots. You could probably also tell that 845 dots is more than 100 dots plus 189 dots, even if you saw all three sets of dots separately.By giving tests like these to kids and adults, scientists are discovering that we develop a sense for large numbers long before we learn how to count or talk. Our brains seem to come equipped with systems for estimating amounts and doing arithmetic, says Elizabeth Spelke. She's a psychologist at Harvard University in Cambridge, Mass. Spelke's work adds to a growing body of research showing that young children and even animals have some inborn sense of number, long before they ever receive a formal math lesson (see "It's a Math World for Animals"). Spelke's goal is to answer one basic question. "What abilities do children have when they start school that support the learning of mathematics?" she asks. The answer, Spelke says, could help teachers find better ways to teach math by tapping into skills that their students bring with them. "The premise of my work is that all learning in school builds on systems we already have," Spelke says. "My goal is to figure out what those systems are, how they work, and how they come together for mathematical thinking." Flash math To add, subtract, multiply, or divide, you need to understand that numbers are symbols that can be used in a variety of settings. The symbol 11, for instance, can refer to your age, the number of days left before vacation, the number of chimes you hear when the clock strikes 11 a.m., or the total number of cookies left over in three different boxes. That's a tricky concept, and the ability to understand numbers as symbols is one thing that separates people from other animals. Unlike other animals, people can figure out exactly what amount you get when you add 32 and 16, without even having to know whether the numbers refer to coins, apples, or anything else. Spelke and her coworkers have done various experiments to find out how well young children can estimate quantities and add large numbers—even before they're taught arithmetic in school. Their most recent attempt to uncover the foundations of our mathematical abilities involved five experiments. In each experiment, preschoolers sat in front of computers watching animations that briefly flashed various sets of dots on the screen. Each set contained from 10 to 58 dots. First, Spelke and her colleagues presented a group of 5-year-olds with one set of blue dots and one set of red dots. The sets of dots appeared one at a time, and they flashed by too quickly to be counted. The results showed that the children were usually able to pick the larger set, even if the dots were different sizes and took up different amounts of space. For the second experiment, the 5-year-olds saw one group of blue dots, which was followed by a second group of blue dots, which was followed by a group of red dots. This setup was more complicated than the first, but the kids were still able to say whether there were more blue dots altogether or red dots, suggesting that they could both add and compare amounts. In the third experiment, the children had to compare a group of dots that they saw to a certain number of tones that they heard. They could still pick the larger group. In the fourth, they successfully chose the largest grouping, given two sets of dots of the same color and one set of tones. These results suggest that kids have an ability to add and compare numbers in different settings. Word problems In the fifth set of experiments, the researchers used words instead of dots or tones to present comparison problems. Here's an example that's like the second experiment with just dots. Suppose you have 15 pennies. Your Mom gives you about 10 more. Your sister has about 40. Who has more pennies: you or your sister? When given in words, the 5-year-olds couldn't answer the question correctly. Most of them, however, could answer the question when it was given as sets of dots. Overall, Spelke concluded that a child's sense of number doesn't depend on his or her ability to use language. Other studies with adults, she says, have shown that we use one part of our brains when doing arithmetic, such as adding 43 plus 72, and another part when estimating and comparing sets of dots. Building blocks So, certain building blocks for learning math seem to be in place before kids start school, Spelke says. It then takes years of math class for the brain to learn how to combine its inborn number sense with its language and other symbolic skills. High grades on algebra tests are the final result. If you're trying hard and still getting B's and C's in math, though, you're not alone. "Learning elementary arithmetic is surprisingly hard for all children," Spelke says. Her research shows, however, that you might be better at certain types of math than you think you are. That should give you hope. "It is both surprising and encouraging to see," Spelke says, "that if you engage kids in thinking about number outside of symbolism, they understand a lot." Even then, most kids need to take classes and do lots of homework to learn arithmetic. The effort is worth it, Spelke says, because without math, our culture couldn't function. "So much of our understanding of the world depends on mathematics," she says. "Math is fundamental to measurement," Spelke says. "It's fundamental to technology. It's fundamental to science."

Wired for Math
Wired for Math








Designed and Powered by HBJamaica.com™